L'utilizzo di gesti complessi come supporto per l’acquisizione di parole nuove durante attività di lettura ad alta voce. Studio pilota con bambini dai 3 ai 5 anni.

Autori

  • Raffaele Dicataldo Research fellow | Department of Developmental Psychology and Socialization | University of Padua |
  • Irene Leo Associate Professor | Department of Developmental Psychology and Socialization | University of Padua |
  • Maja Roch Associate Professor | Department of Developmental Psychology and Socialization | University of Padua |

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.7347/EdL-01-2023-03

Parole chiave:

Vocabolario, Apprendimento, Gesti, Cognizione corporea, Bambini in età prescolare

Abstract

La lettura ad alta voce è una delle attività più importanti che genitori, insegnanti e/o educatori possono fare con i bambini in diversi contesti educativi, poiché promuove lo sviluppo del linguaggio orale. Negli ultimi anni, diverse evidenze suggeriscono che integrare il movimento nei compiti cognitivi possa essere efficace per l’apprendimento grazie ai suoi effetti cognitivi e fisiologici. Secondo la teoria della Cognizione Incarnata, la mente è connessa al corpo, così come i pensieri, le emozioni, le azioni e le sensazioni. Se la cognizione coinvolge il corpo, è ragionevole supporre che anche l’apprendimento del linguaggio lo faccia. Il progetto di ricerca “Letture in Movimento” mira a indagare se l’esposizione multimodale (contesto linguistico supportato dal movimento) a parole target trovate nelle storie lette ai bambini durante le attività di lettura ad alta voce supporti l’acquisizione di nuove parole nei bambini in età prescolare. Il presente progetto pilota ha coinvolto 50 bambini (15 femmine) con un’età media di 57 mesi (ds = 10.4). Prima dell’intervento, le competenze lessicali e motorie dei partecipanti sono state valutate attraverso il TFL e il PDMS-2. L’intervento è stato sviluppato in 4 sessioni. In ciascuna sessione, è stata letta ad alta voce una storia contenente 6 parole target: 2 parole accompagnate da gesti non associati al significato, 2 accompagnate da gesti associati al significato e 2 senza gesti. La capacità dei bambini di riconoscere le parole target è stata valutata a T0 e T2 attraverso due test ad hoc. I risultati di questo studio pilota indicano una facilitazione nell’acquisizione di nuove parole durante le attività di lettura ad alta voce se queste sono accompagnate da un gesto semanticamente correlato, supportando l’ipotesi che integrare attività motorie in un compito di apprendimento linguistico sembri particolarmente efficace per l’apprendimento.

Downloads

I dati di download non sono ancora disponibili.

Riferimenti bibliografici

Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(4), 577-660.

Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Infant gaze following and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth through two years of age: A longitudinal, growth curve modeling study. Journal of Child Language, 35(1), 207-220.

Chomsky, N. (2009). On the Goals of Linguistic Theory. In Syntactic Structures (pp. 49-60). De Gruyter Mouton.

Decety, J., Grezes, J., Costes, N., Perani, D., Jeannerod, M., Procyk, E., & Fazio, F. (1997). Brain activity during observation of actions. Influence of action content and subject's strategy. Brain: a Journal of Neurology, 120(10), 1763-1777.

Deckner, D. F., Adamson, L. B., & Bakeman, R. (2006). Child and maternal contributions to shared reading: Effects on language and literacy development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27(1), 31-41.

Dicataldo, R., & Roch, M. (2020). Are the effects of variation in quantity of daily bilingual exposure and socioeconomic status on language and cognitive abilities independent in preschool children?. International journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(12), 4570.

Dicataldo, R., & Roch, M. (2022). How Does Toddlers’ Engagement in Literacy Activities Influence Their Language Abilities?. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(1), 526.

Dicataldo, R., Rowe, M. L., & Roch, M. (2022). “Let’s Read Together”, a Parent-Focused Intervention on Dialogic Book Reading to Improve Early Language and Literacy Skills in Preschool Children. Children, 9(8), 1149-1162.

Dickinson, D. K., Griffith, J. A., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2012). How reading books fosters language development around the world. Child Development Research, 1-15.

Duursma, E., Augustyn, M., & Zuckerman, B. (2008). Reading aloud to children: the evidence. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 93(7), 554-557

Glenberg, A. M. (2008). Embodiment for education. In Handbook of Cognitive Science (pp. 355-372). Elsevier.

Goldin-Meadow, S., Nusbaum, H., Kelly, S. D., & Wagner, S. (2001). Explaining math: Gesturing lightens the load. Psychological Science, 12(6), 516-522.

Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (2003). The early catastrophe. Education Review, 17(1), 110-130.

Hillman, C. H., Castelli, D. M., & Buck, S. M. (2005). Aerobic fitness and neurocognitive function in healthy preadolescent children. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 37, 1967–1974.

Hoff, E., & Naigles, L. (2002). How children use input to acquire a lexicon. Child Development, 73(2), 418-433.

Hoff, E., Core, C., Place, S., Rumiche, R., Señor, M., & Parra, M. (2012). Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 39(1), 1-27.

Hogan, T., Bridges, M. S., Justice, L. M., & Cain, K. (2011). Increasing higher-level language skills to improve reading comprehension.

Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2008). Visible embodiment: Gestures as simulated action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 495-514.

Huffman, L. C., Mehlinger, S. L., Kerivan, A. S., Cavanaugh, D. A., Lippitt, J., & Moyo, O. (2001). Off to a Good Start: Research on the Risk Factors for Early School Problems and Selected Federal Policies Affecting Children's Social and Emotional Development and Their Readiness for School.

Iverson, J. M. (2010). Developing language in a developing body: The relationship between motor development and language development. Journal of Child Language, 37(2), 229-261.

Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2006). The tortuous route from genes to behavior: A neuroconstructivist approach. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 6(1), 9-17.

Lindemann, O., Stenneken, P., Van Schie, H. T., & Bekkering, H. (2006). Semantic activation in action planning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(3), 633.

Manti, F., Giovannone, F., Ciancaleoni, M., De Vita, G., Fioriello, F., Gigliotti, F., & Sogos, C. (2023). Psychometric Properties and Validation of the Italian Version of Ages & Stages Questionnaires Third Edition. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 20(6), 5014.

Maouene, J., Hidaka, S., & Smith, L. B. (2008). Body parts and early‐learned verbs. Cognitive Science, 32(7), 1200-1216.

Maouene, J., Sethuraman, N., Maoene, M., & Smith, L. B. (2010, August). An Embodied Account of Argument Structure Development. In Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (Vol. 36, No. 1, pp. 261-275).

Mavilidi, M. F., Okely, A. D., Chandler, P., Cliff, D. P., & Paas, F. (2015). Effects of integrated physical exercises and gestures on preschool children’s foreign language vocabulary learning. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 413-426.

Merton, R. K. (1973). The sociology of science: Theoretical and empirical investigations. University of Chicago press.

Novack, M. A., Congdon, E. L., Hemani-Lopez, N., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2014). From action to abstraction: Using the hands to learn math. Psychological Science, 25(4), 903-910.

Opel, A., Ameer, S. S., & Aboud, F. E. (2009). The effect of preschool dialogic reading on vocabulary among rural Bangladeshi children. International Journal of Educational Research, 48(1), 12-20.

Paas, F., & Sweller, J. (2012). An evolutionary upgrade of cognitive load theory: Using the human motor system and collaboration to support the learning of complex cognitive tasks. Educational Psychology Review, 24, 27-45.

Ping, R., & Goldin‐Meadow, S. (2010). Gesturing saves cognitive resources when talking about nonpresent objects. Cognitive Science, 34(4), 602-619.

Rahn, N. L., Coogle, C. G., & Storie, S. (2016). Preschool children’s use of thematic vocabulary during dialogic reading and activity-based intervention. The Journal of Special Education, 50(2), 98-108.

Ramey, C. T., & Ramey, S. L. (1998). Early intervention and early experience. American psychologist, 53(2), 109-131.

Romeo, R. R., Leonard, J. A., Robinson, S. T., West, M. R., Mackey, A. P., Rowe, M. L., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2018). Beyond the 30-million-word gap: Children’s conversational exposure is associated with language-related brain function. Psychological Science, 29(5), 700-710.

Rowe, M. L., Leech, K. A., & Cabrera, N. (2017). Going beyond input quantity: Wh‐questions matter for toddlers' language and cognitive development. Cognitive Science, 41, 162-179.

Sclaunich, M. (2012). La lettura ad alta voce come possibile strumento per promuovere l'incontro tra bambino e libro fin dalla prima infanzia. Lifelong Lifewide Learning, 8(20), 1-11.

Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J. A. (2002). Parental involvement in the development of children’s reading skill: A five‐year longitudinal study. Child Development, 73(2), 445-460.

Sim, S., & Berthelsen, D. (2014). Shared book reading by parents with young children: Evidence-based practice. Australasian Journal of Early Childhood, 39(1), 50-55.

Thordardottir, E. (2015). The relationship between bilingual exposure and morphosyntactic development. International Journal of Speech-language Pathology, 17(2), 97-114.

Tomporowski, P. D., Davis, C. L., Miller, P. H., & Naglieri, J. A. (2008). Exercise and children’s intelligence, cognition, and academic achievement. Educational Psychology review, 20, 111-131.

Toumpaniari, K., Loyens, S., Mavilidi, M. F., & Paas, F. (2015). Preschool children’s foreign language vocabulary learning by embodying words through physical activity and gesturing. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 445-456.

Trofatter, C., Kontra, C., Beilock, S., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2015). Gesturing has a larger impact on problem-solving than action, even when action is accompanied by words. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30(3), 251-260.

Vicari, S., Marotta, L., & Luci, A. (2007). TFL Test Fono-lessicale: Valutazione delle abilità lessicali in età prescolare. Edizioni Erickson.

Walsh, B. A. (2008). Review of Research: Quantity, Quality, Children's Characteristics, and Vocabulary Learning. Childhood Education, 84(3), 163-166.

Zwaan, R. A., & Taylor, L. J. (2006). Seeing, acting, understanding: motor resonance in language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(1), 1-15.

##submission.downloads##

Pubblicato

30-06-2023

Come citare

Dicataldo, R., Leo, I., & Roch, M. (2023). L’utilizzo di gesti complessi come supporto per l’acquisizione di parole nuove durante attività di lettura ad alta voce. Studio pilota con bambini dai 3 ai 5 anni. Effetti Di Lettura / Effects of Reading, 2(1), 032–043. https://doi.org/10.7347/EdL-01-2023-03

Fascicolo

Sezione

Articoli